CNV boundaries were estimated based on the probe (log2) ratio inf

CNV boundaries were estimated based on the probe (log2) ratio information from tilling array CGH. PCR primers were then designed to amplify the breakpoints of five de novo deletions. PCR was performed on genomic DNA from all members of the trio. PCR products were sequenced by the Sanger selleck screening library method using both forward and reverse primers specific for each de novo deletion. We examined whether genes impacted by de novo CNVs in SCZ, BD, and controls were enriched for specific functional

categories. In addition, functional categories found to be enriched within each diagnostic group were interrogated in rare CNVs from large independent cases control data sets including 8,290 SCZ, 2,777 BD, and 7,431 controls. For gene set enrichment analysis, we used 39 de novo CNVs including nine in SCZ, www.selleckchem.com/products/gsk126.html ten in BD, four in our controls, and an additional 16 CNVs detected in a previous study by Levy et al. (2011) in an independent set of control subjects using the same array platform. We prefer to use only de novo CNVs as a control set. Naturally occurring variants in the population

do not make the ideal control set for this analysis because the gene content of these CNVs is shaped by natural selection and is not likely to be representative of random mutation. Gene set enrichment analyses was performed on the sets of genes impacted by de novo CNVs in SCZ, BD, and controls. The primary step was performed using “DAVID Bioinformatics Resources 6.7” website (http://david.abcc.ncifcrf.gov/) using Gene Ontology terms—biological processes (GO_BP), cellular components (GO_CC), and molecular functions (GO_MF)—including KEGG, Phosphoprotein phosphatase BioCarta, BBID, and Panther pathway databases and by excluding pathway results containing < 3 CNV genes. We selected the nonredundant pathways from DAVID with p value < 0.05 for further analysis by permutation-based test. Based on analysis using DAVID, eight categories were enriched among de novo CNVs in SCZ (Table 4), seven categories were enriched among de novo CNVs in BD (Table 5), and nine categories were enriched among de novo CNVs in controls (Table S7). The enrichment test performed

within the DAVID software does not correct for certain biases of CNVs toward certain functional classes of genes and large genes in particular. In order to correct for these biases we applied two permutation-based tests to the pathways found to be enriched by DAVID. First, we performed a case-only permutation-based test by constructing empirical null distributions that took the CNV size distribution and gene number into account. We randomly placed 10,000 sets of CNVs (same number of events, size distribution) throughout the genome. Placement on any autosome was allowed, but we sampled such that placement on chromosomes was weighted in proportion to the total number of de novo CNVs observed on the respective chromosome.

, 2001) Associative odor learning modifies piriform cortical odo

, 2001). Associative odor learning modifies piriform cortical odor-evoked activity as assessed with single-unit recording (Calu et al., 2007, Roesch et al., 2007 and Zinyuk et al., selleck chemicals 2001), ensemble recording (J. Chapuis and D.A. Wilson, 2010, Soc. Neurosci., abstract; Kadohisa and Wilson, 2006), local field potential recording (Chapuis et al., 2009 and Martin et al., 2006), 2-deoxyglucose uptake (Moriceau and Sullivan, 2004), and c-fos immune reactivity

( Datiche et al., 2001). These evoked response changes may reflect synaptic or neural plasticity within the olfactory cortex itself ( Brosh and Barkai, 2004 and Saar and Barkai, 2003) or reflect changes in functional connectivity within the larger network of which the olfactory cortex is a part ( Martin et al., 2007 and Martin et al., 2004). For example, odor learning modifies the synaptic strength of both olfactory bulb and orbitofrontal cortex projections to the piriform cortex ( Cohen selleck chemical et al., 2008). As described above, this rich experience-dependent plasticity may be involved not only in associating odors with context or outcome, but also in helping modify sensory acuity for the familiar or learned odor (J. Chapuis

and D.A. Wilson, 2010, Soc. Neurosci., abstract; Chen et al., 2011 and Kadohisa and Wilson, 2006). In addition to experience-dependent changes in functional connectivity of the olfactory cortex, connectivity is also influenced by behavioral state. Single-unit and local field potential responses to odor in the anterior piriform cortex are greatly reduced during slow-wave sleep (Barnes et al., 2011, Murakami et al., 2005 and Wilson, 2010) and certain stages of anesthesia (Fontanini and Bower, 2005). Although there is a circadian rhythm in olfactory sensitivity in rodents (Granados-Fuentes et al., 2006), the sleep-related cortical hyposensitivity is rapid, is selective to slow-wave sleep and not REM and does not appear in the olfactory bulb (Barnes et al., 2011, Murakami

et al., 2005 and Wilson, 2010). Piriform cortical MRIP activity during slow-wave sleep is dominated by sharp waves (Manabe et al., 2011), similar to those observed in hippocampus (Buzsáki, 1986), and single-unit activity during these sharp waves is shaped by recent odor experience (Wilson, 2010). This latter observation may suggest an opportunity for odor “replay” during slow-wave sleep while the cortex is otherwise hyporesponsive to afferent input. Such replay could help consolidate intracortical association fiber plasticity underlying memory of new odor objects (Wilson, 2010), as well as send a strong excitatory feedback to olfactory bulb that could be critical for survival of odor-specific populations of newborn granule cells (Manabe et al., 2011).

To delineate the metabolic shifts underlying our observations we

To delineate the metabolic shifts underlying our observations we performed NMR-based serum metabolomics analysis. However, given the significant reduction in serum fatty acids and glucose in the EX group, surprisingly subtle changes were observed in serum metabolites levels. No change in leptin or adiponectin concentrations was observed after exercise training, whilst dieting significantly find more decreased leptin and increased adiponectin levels, indicating that the reduction in serum glucose and free fatty acids did not result from insulin sensitizing effects of adipokines.36 Exercise training was not associated with

changes in beta hydroxybutyrate to acetoacetate ratio or lactate to pyruvate ratio which are indicators of redox state in mitochondria,37 suggesting

that the beneficial effects of aerobic selleckchem exercise on glucose and lipid metabolism was not ascribed to reduced reactive oxygen species production. Exercise resulted in increased serum phenylalanine and glycine and, to a lesser significant extent, tyrosine and histidine concentrations. Increased levels of serum phenylalanine and tyrosine have been associated with obesity and insulin resistance in previous studies,38 whereas increased serum glycine has been associated with increased insulin sensitivity and fatty acid oxidation.39 Thus, reductions in insulin resistance and serum free fatty acids in the EX group may have resulted from recovery of serum glycine concentrations and subsequent increase in mitochondrial function. Moreover, it has been shown that, in sedentary subjects, greater metabolic stress results in adaptation manifested by improved muscular mitochondrial biogenesis and enhanced fatty acid oxidation,40 which thereby may accelerate fatty acid removal from circulation.41 Calpain Therefore, it is plausible that the mitochondrial adaptation to progressive exercise training was responsible for the reduction in free fatty acids and the subsequent reduction in insulin resistance in

the present study. The strength of our randomized trial study design reached the methodological requirements of physical activity trials42 to provide reliable and valid evidence on the effects of exercise. Our design also focused on moderate progressive aerobic exercise training because this is what is recommended in most public health guidelines for adults.43 All the samples were analyzed in the same laboratory at the same time by a same person. However, the current study is limited by the homogeneous subjects consisting Finnish women. Therefore, our results may not be transferrable to the general population. The fact that we used bioimpedance instead of dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry or magnetic resonance imaging to define body composition may predispose our results to measurement error.44 Furthermore, VO2max was defined by bicycle ergometer, while exercise training consisted of Nordic walking.

, 2008) Further, we found Egfr

expression is strongly mo

, 2008). Further, we found Egfr

expression is strongly modulated by MEK signaling ( Table 1). EGFR has been shown to increase its expression during late cortical development and promotes progenitor gliogenic competence ( Viti et al., 2003). Finally, it is highly likely that MEK acts through epigenetic mechanisms to regulate transcription of multiple genes related to glial differentiation. Indeed a prior study strongly implicated modulation of H3 methylation as an important mechanism of FGF signaling in the cell fate switch that allowed glial differentiation ( Song and Ghosh, 2004). Analysis of epigenetic regulation will be an important area for future investigation. Importantly, gliogenesis has been assessed in detail in mouse models of human syndromes due to RAF/MEK/ERK cascade overactivation. Work in a mouse model of neurofibromatosis OSI-744 clinical trial type1 (NF1) has shown a dramatic increase in brain gliogenesis MK-1775 solubility dmso and decreased neurogenesis (Hegedus et al., 2007), findings that are

very much in line with the results reported here. A study using a Costello syndrome H-RAS active mutant construct also showed a similar phenotype (Paquin et al., 2009). In both of these syndromes, gene mutations lead to overactivation of RAF/MEK/ERK signaling and the phenotypes are entirely consistent with our findings. Another RAS/MAPK syndrome, Noonan’s syndrome, typically results from mutations in SHP-2, an upstream modifier of the RAF/MEK/ERK cascade. Our results are not in line with the concept that a SHP-2-MEK/ERK cascade is essential for neurogenesis and suppresses gliogenesis as has been reported previously (Gauthier et al., 2007; Ke et al., 2007). Although reasons for these differing interpretations are not entirely clear, it is important to note that SHP-2 regulates

several signaling cascades in addition to the RAS-MAPK pathway. Additional effects of SHP-2 regulation include activation of PI3K-AKT pathway and inactivation of JAK-STAT3 pathway (Coskun et al., 2007; Feng, 2007; Neel et al., 2003). Thus, the reported effects in Shp-2 deficient animals heptaminol may be due to abnormalities in several pathways. Whatever the explanation for divergent results related to SHP-2, our results are definitive as to the gliogenic functions mediated by MEK. Astrocytes are thought to have critical functions in the postnatal brain related to neuronal support and synaptic function. However, few prior studies have produced brains where astrocyte number has been dramatically reduced during development. We have defined several important in vivo consequences of regulating glial number in our study. First, we noticed that Mek1,2\Nes mice are acallosal due to absence of midline astroglia. Interestingly, the Fgfr1f/f;NesCre conditional mutant shows a similar phenotype.

These are, in fact, not instantaneous, although their kinetics ar

These are, in fact, not instantaneous, although their kinetics are sub-millisecond (Mennerick and Matthews, 1996) and thus are effectively instantaneous at the timescale that we modeled. A more biophysical model would also translate this approximation into a kinetic model. In the model, separate control over the internal mean and higher-order statistics allowed us to conclude that adaptation depends on the mean input to the kinetics block (Figure 6). We therefore predict that adaptation at the bipolar synaptic terminal depends only on

the mean value of the internal calcium concentration. However, in an experiment, an attempt to separately BAY 73-4506 price control the mean and variance of the bipolar membrane potential or calcium concentration using visual stimuli would produce luminance adaptation, which can occur in as little as 0.1 s (Baylor and Hodgkin, 1974). A definitive experimental test of the prediction that the bipolar cell terminal adapts to the mean of the rectified membrane potential would bypass photoreceptors, directly manipulating the membrane potential

or calcium concentration at the synaptic terminal. Previous results indicate that adaptation to statistics beyond mean luminance is controlled primarily by standard deviation (Bonin et al., 2006). Our finding selleck kinase inhibitor that contrast adaptation is controlled by the mean of an internal variable is not in conflict with this result. Because the initial filter combines multiple samples from the stimulus, due to the central limit theorem this will reduce the effects of higher-order moments of the stimulus, making the filtered stimulus more Gaussian. Thus, the standard deviation of the stimulus will have the largest control over the mean signal after it passes through the threshold nonlinearity. Because thresholds are common in the nervous system, it is likely that a signal with changing

variance will be transformed to a signal with a changing mean, giving rise to the commonly observed properties of variance adaptation. In the model, changes in the timescale of slow adaptation are produced by the variable rate constant of slow recovery, ksr, which we found to be proportional to the contrast. Although our studies used a fixed time interval, this timescale of adaptation can change to match the Thiamine-diphosphate kinase timescale of changes in the stimulus contrast ( Wark et al., 2009). Such plasticity of adaptive timescale would not automatically occur in our current model because such behavior would require ksr to depend on the timescale of contrast changes. If, as we propose, changes in ksr reflects the calcium dependence of slow vesicle mobility ( Gomis et al., 1999), this would predict that this mechanism reflects an inference about the recent timescale of changes in stimulus contrast. Our stimuli had a constant mean intensity and, thus, avoided luminance adaptation, which appears to be independent from contrast adaptation (Mante et al., 2005).

The recent generation of a conditional KO mouse in which both sta

The recent generation of a conditional KO mouse in which both stargazin and γ-7 are deleted shows that the additional removal of γ-7 further reduces

PC climbing fiber responses to ∼10% of wild-type, thus implicating γ-7 in mediating some synaptic targeting in the absence of stargazin. Phenotypically, the stargazin/γ-7 double KO appears find more to exhibit more severe ataxia than stargazin KOs ( Yamazaki et al., 2010). The impact that these various TARP deletions may have on forms of cerebellar synaptic plasticity, such as LTD at parallel fiber-PC synapses, remains to be seen. Cerebellar stellate cells (SCs) and basket cells (BCs) are small interneurons that reside in the molecular layer, receive parallel fiber input, and mediate feedforward inhibition onto PCs.

Recent work has shown that SCs from stargazer mice exhibit a profound loss in synaptic AMPARs but preservation of extrasynaptic receptors ( Jackson and Nicoll, 2011), underscoring a possible role for different TARP family members in the subcellular compartmentalization of AMPARs in neurons ( Rouach et al., 2005, Inamura et al., 2006, Menuz and Nicoll, 2008 and Ferrario et al., 2011). In addition, parallel fiber-SC synapses exhibit a unique form of synaptic plasticity ( Liu and Cull-Candy, 2000) that is compromised in stargazer mice ( Jackson and Nicoll, 2011). Thus far, Bergmann glial cells (BGCs) are the only glial cells that have been studied in any Calpain detail in the context of TARPs. BGCs are essential for the development and function of the cerebellar cortex (Bellamy, Veliparib nmr 2006) and expression of calcium-permeable AMPARs (Iino et al., 2001).

Interestingly, BGCs express both TARP γ-4 and TARP γ-5 (Tomita et al., 2003, Fukaya et al., 2005 and Lein et al., 2007). Although γ-4 is the predominant TARP expressed in the brain during development, its expression persists in adult BGCs (Tomita et al., 2003). BGCs have been used as a model system for examining AMPAR subunit-specific trafficking and gating by γ-5. The AMPAR properties of BGCs closely match those of heterologous cells in which GluA4 is coexpressed with γ-5, suggesting that γ-5 has a functional role in modulating glutamatergic transmission in BGCs (Soto et al., 2009). In addition to profound ataxia and dyskinesia, stargazer mice exhibit seizure activity characterized by SWDs, qualitatively similar to human absence epilepsy ( Noebels et al., 1990). To investigate the cellular mechanisms that account for this aspect of the stargazer phenotype, several studies have focused on the neocortex and thalamus. Dysregulation of excitability and synchrony within recurrent corticothalamic loops has been implicated in the origin of absence seizures ( Huguenard and McCormick, 2007 and Beenhakker and Huguenard, 2009).

, 2012 and Hille, 2001) Ion channel proteins form holes in membr

, 2012 and Hille, 2001). Ion channel proteins form holes in membranes that open and close in response to various chemical and electrical stimuli. These structures allow cells to tap into the energy stored selleck chemical in transmembrane ionic gradients to generate the electrical signals that race through our nerves and muscles. In 1988, when Neuron launched, it published 21 papers devoted to some aspect of ion channel research in its first year. These covered topics spanning from basic channel biophysics to the behavior of channels in complex systems. In reflecting on the questions that motivated ion channel research 25 years ago, it is striking that the

spirit, if not the details, of the studies exemplified

in Neuron’s inaugural year mark many of the same questions that occupy the field today. These include: what is the physical nature of a channel ( Auld et al., 1988, Ballivet et al., 1988, Deneris et al., 1988, Levitan et al., 1988, Lotan et al., 1988, Rudy et al., 1988 and Timpe selleck products et al., 1988)? How do ions and pharmacological tools interact with channel pores ( MacKinnon et al., 1988, Miller, 1988 and Miller et al., 1988)? Where are particular channels expressed ( Harris et al., 1988, Siegel, 1988, Wang et al., 1988, Wisden et al., 1988 and Wollner et al., 1988) and how is this regulated by development or electrical activity ( Goldman et al., 1988 and Hendry and Jones, 1988)? many How do channels respond to manipulations in diverse types of excitable cells ( Doerner and Alger, 1988, Haydon and Man-Son-Hing, 1988, Lechleiter et al., 1988, Lipscombe et al., 1988, Maricq and Korenbrot, 1988, Pfaffinger et al., 1988 and Yakel and Jackson, 1988)? At the silver anniversary of the journal, we reflect on how much the field has changed, how certain classes of questions persist, and highlight some key open questions that rest upon the major achievements

of the past quarter century but that still represent areas of great opportunity for discovery. The ion channel field is vast and it would take a book to do it justice. Great progress has been made in understanding how channels “gate” their pores. To capture some of this excitement in a short space, we focus on three areas of phenomenal advancement that frame key unaddressed problems: (1) the transformation from cartoon to three dimensions of our understanding of the molecular nature of channels, (2) a tale of one mechanism that is central to understanding neural signaling, voltage sensing, and (3) how the complicated, multicomponent protein complexes of channels are assembled and delivered to the right place in the cell. These basic issues permeate the biological functions of all ion channels and understanding such facets of channel biology remains critical for unraveling how channels operate in normal and disease states.

, 2012 and Marenco et al , 2011) Multiple lines of evidence sugg

, 2012 and Marenco et al., 2011). Multiple lines of evidence suggest that gamma-band oscillations are reduced during the execution of cognitive tasks in schizophrenia (Haenschel et al., 2009, Hirano et al., 2008, Spencer et al., 2003 and Uhlhaas et al., 2006). However, recent studies indicate that medication-naive, first-episode, and chronic patients with schizophrenia show elevated gamma-band power in resting state (Kikuchi et al., 2011 and Spencer, 2011). Thus, cortical rhythm abnormalities find more in schizophrenia seem to include abnormal increases in baseline power as well as deficits in task-related oscillations (Uhlhaas and Singer, 2012). Baseline increases in gamma oscillations are consistent with increases in

the excitatory/inhibitory ratio of neurons (Yizhar et al., 2011), as observed here in conditional Erbb4 mutants. Consistently, loss of NR1 receptors from PV+ interneurons leads to increased gamma-band oscillations in both anesthetized and behaving mice ( Carlén et al., 2012 and Korotkova et al., 2010). Remarkably, deletion

of NR1 in PV+ interneurons also results in a significant reduction of theta oscillations ( Carlén et al., 2012 and Korotkova et al., 2010), which reflects the cellular specificity of both models. Abnormal coupling between the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex have been observed in schizophrenia patients (Ford et al., 2002, Heckers et al., 1998, Lawrie et al., 2002 and Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2005). Mice carrying the 22q11.2 microdeletion, a mutation Birinapant nmr associated with high risk for schizophrenia, also show disrupted synchrony between the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex (Sigurdsson et al., 2010). Our current findings, which reveal abnormal

hippocampal-prefrontal synchrony in conditional Erbb4 mutants, reinforce enough the notion that genetic susceptibility to schizophrenia is strongly linked to deficient functional connectivity between temporal and frontal regions of the cortex. Finally, impaired synchrony between the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex is associated with working memory deficits (Sigurdsson et al., 2010), as shown here in Lhx6-Cre;Erbb4F/F mutants. Working memory deficits have been previously observed in nervous system-specific Erbb4 mice and in PVCre;Erbb4F/F conditional mutants ( Golub et al., 2004 and Wen et al., 2010), which suggest that impaired function of fast-spiking interneurons is associated with these defects. Beyond cognition, loss of Erbb4 from fast-spiking interneurons also impacts many different aspects of behavior that have been previously associated with schizophrenia. Lhx6-Cre;Erbb4F/F mice were generated by breeding Lhx6-Cre mice ( Fogarty et al., 2007) with mice carrying loxP-flanked (F) Erbb4 alleles ( Golub et al., 2004) and sometimes with Rosa26 Reporter CAG-boosted EGFP (RCE) mice ( Sousa et al., 2009). For most experiments, controls include mice carrying wild-type and Lhx6-Cre alleles.

, 1995) In the motor domain, reorganization of M1 motor maps (Mo

, 1995). In the motor domain, reorganization of M1 motor maps (Monfils et al., 2005 and Nudo et al., 1996a) and changes

in spine turnover (Xu et al., 2009) were found after motor skill acquisition (Figure 1A). Longitudinal studies in human subjects using MRI showed that new motor skill acquisition can result in map plasticity (Pascual-Leone et al., 2005 and Pascual-Leone et al., 1995) and increased cortical thickness (Draganski et al., 2004) (Figure 1B). More complete elucidation of sensory and motor neural circuits in the normal and disease states is required for understanding the cellular basis of cortical map plasticity and for developing more precise and effective plasticity-based therapies. Activity is the main driving force for adaptive changes in the nervous system. While persistent changes in activity levels may lead to re-adjustment SP600125 solubility dmso of the neuronal and synaptic components that allow homeostatic regulation of neural circuit functions (Turrigiano,

2012), much interest selleck chemicals in the past decades has been focused on activity-dependent plasticity that sets neural circuits into new functional states. Such plasticity at synaptic and neuronal levels provides the basis for the development of neural circuits in the first place, and it endows the capacity for neural circuits to perform the signal processing underlying many cognitive functions. The complex molecular and cellular machinery for the control of neurotransmitter release and postsynaptic responses makes the synapse the most sensitive site for activity-induced modifications in the nervous system. Short-term synaptic modification plays an immediate role in adapting and extending the signal-processing capability of neural

circuits (Abbott et al., 1997 and Zucker and Regehr, 2002), whereas long-term modification provides the basis for learning and memory functions. many The discoveries of rapid activity-induced LTP and LTD in various systems (Malenka and Nicoll, 1993) and the ease in studying these phenomena in brain slices have triggered extensive studies of their underlying cellular and molecular mechanisms. It is now clear that nearly all central synapses exhibit both short-term and long-term plasticity in response to repetitive synaptic activities, through changes in either presynaptic transmitter release or postsynaptic responses to transmitters—or both (Malenka and Bear, 2004). Different patterns of neuronal activities may activate distinct forms of LTP and LTD, and the induction and expression mechanisms may differ among various types of synapses and at different developmental stages. Please see Perspective by Huganir and Nicoll (2013) in this issue for more information. It is generally recognized that a brief high-frequency synaptic activation often results in LTP while prolonged low-frequency activation leads to LTD.

, 2008 and Zenisek et al , 2000]) In WT astrocytes (data not sho

, 2008 and Zenisek et al., 2000]). In WT astrocytes (data not shown) and in Tnf−/− astrocytes incubated with TNFα, ( Figure 4E) the two pools underwent exocytosis in a clear biphasic temporal sequence: during the first phase (0–400 ms) most of the fusing vesicles belonged to the “resident” pool (80.6%, n = 7 cells), whereas during the second phase (500 ms–2 s), to the “newcomers” pool (82.5%). This temporal segregation reflects the different readiness to fusion of the two pools, in particular the fact that most “resident” vesicles, contrary to “newcomers,”

have already undergone the docking steps and are ready for fusion (i.e., are functionally docked [ Ohara-Imaizumi et al., 2007 and Toonen et al., 2006]). However, in Tnf−/− see more astrocytes,

the situation was very different. Events attributable to “residents” decreased in percentage (20% instead of 40%; n = 3680 vesicle fusions analyzed, n = 7 cells). Moreover, importantly, events due to “residents” BMS-777607 nmr and “newcomers” occurred randomly, without the expected temporal segregation. This indicates that even the residual “resident” pool seen in Figure 4A is defective in Tnf−/− astrocytes, because it is not ready/competent to fuse. Most likely, these vesicles dock only transiently and, like all the others, from in the absence of TNFα are hampered in reaching the stage of functional docking

allowing them to undergo rapid fusion ( Toonen et al., 2006). We conclude that constitutive TNFα is necessary for the correct reception of glutamatergic vesicles to release sites, a precondition for efficient exocytosis upon stimulation. In parallel TIRF experiments, we studied local submembrane Ca2+ events, previously shown to be temporally locked to exocytic events (Marchaland et al., 2008). Indeed, in WT astrocytes, 2MeSADP stimulation induced a burst of submembrane Ca2+ events whose temporal pattern mirrored the one of VGLUT1-pHluorin fusion events, with two peaks of Ca2+ events, each one slightly preceding the corresponding peak of vesicular fusions (Figure 4B, inset). Importantly, and in full agreement with the observations in situ, this pattern was totally preserved in Tnf−/− astrocytes ( Figure 4C, inset), further confirming that TNFα does not act on the coupling between GPCR and [Ca2+]i elevation, and indicating that this step of gliotransmission can be perfectly normal while the downstream signaling is dramatically defective.